NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Slocum, Jeremy Y.; Merriman, William E. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2018
From an early age, children show a tendency to map novel labels onto unfamiliar rather than familiar kinds of objects. Accounts of this tendency have not addressed whether children develop a metacognitive representation of what they are doing. In 3 experiments (each N = 48), preschoolers received a test of the "metacognitive disambiguation…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Preschool Children, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hartin, Travis L.; Stevenson, Colleen M.; Merriman, William E. – Language Learning and Development, 2016
The ability to judge the limits of one's own knowledge may play an important role in knowledge acquisition. The current study tested the prediction that preschoolers would judge the limits of their lexical knowledge more accurately if they were first exposed to a few objects of contrasting familiarity. Such preexposure was hypothesized to increase…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Young Children, Knowledge Level, Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lipowski, Stacy L.; Merriman, William E. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2011
According to the dual criterion account of early linguistic judgment (Merriman & Lipko, 2008), preschool-aged children who possess more efficient object memory processes should also be more accurate judges of whether various objects have known names. In support of this claim, both the accuracy of object recognition and the speed of object…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Memory, Early Childhood Education, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Merriman, William E.; Lipko, Amanda R. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Preschool-age children were hypothesized to use one of two criteria, cue recognition or target generation, to make several linguistic judgments. When deciding whether a word is one they know, for example, some were expected to consider whether they recognized its sound form (cue recognition), whereas others were expected to consider whether a…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Metalinguistics, Semantics, Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Merriman, William E.; Stevenson, Colleen M. – Child Development, 1997
Used new test to determine whether 24-month olds interpret novel words in accordance with Mutual Exclusivity Bias. Found that when asked to select exemplars of a familiar noun, they avoided objects from previously read story in which novel nouns were used as atypical exemplars of familiar noun. When pronouns and proper names replaced novel nouns,…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Measures (Individuals), Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Merriman, William E.; Schuster, Joneen M. – Child Development, 1991
The tendency of two year olds to select an unfamiliar over a familiar object was less when they were asked to choose between items than to identify a referent for an unfamiliar name. This result suggests that children have both an attraction for novel items and a tendency to honor lexical principles. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Familiarity, Language Acquisition, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Marazita, John M.; Merriman, William E. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Because of its potential importance for word learning, children's judgment of whether they know names for objects was investigated. In Study 1, judgment accuracy was at or near ceiling in about two-thirds of 4-year-olds, and covaried with judgment of word familiarity and with justifying novel name mapping in terms of avoidance of name overlap. The…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Age Differences, Vocabulary Development, Intelligence