NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Researchers1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Preschool and…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 16 to 30 of 35 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Smith, Craig E.; Harris, Paul L. – Social Development, 2012
Experimental studies of children's responses to apologies often present participants with hypothetical scenarios. This article reports on an experimental study of children's reactions to experiencing an actual disappointment and subsequent apology. Participants (ages four to seven) were told that another child was supposed to share some attractive…
Descriptors: Models, Psychological Patterns, Interpersonal Relationship, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bascandziev, Igor; Harris, Paul L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Young children seem to operate under the assumption that objects always fall in a straight vertical line. When asked to search for a ball dropped down an S-shaped opaque tube, they repeatedly search directly below. Hood proposed that children have difficulty in inhibiting their prepotent expectation that objects fall in a straight line (Hood,…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Young Children, Physics, Scientific Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chen, Eva E.; Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Harris, Paul L. – Child Development, 2013
Children prefer to learn from informants in consensus with one another. However, no research has examined whether this preference exists across cultures, and whether the race of the informants impacts that preference. In 2 studies, one hundred thirty-six 4- to 7-year-old European American and Taiwanese children demonstrated a systematic preference…
Descriptors: Trust (Psychology), Preferences, Young Children, Cross Cultural Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kim, Sunae; Kalish, Charles W.; Harris, Paul L. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Prior work shows that children can make inductive inferences about objects based on their labels rather than their appearance (Gelman, 2003). A separate line of research shows that children's trust in a speaker's label is selective. Children accept labels from a reliable speaker over an unreliable speaker (e.g., Koenig & Harris, 2005). In the…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Inferences, Classification, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Kinzler, Katherine D.; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Past research provides evidence that children use at least 2 potentially competing strategies when choosing informants: they attend to informants' past accuracy and to their social identity (e.g., their status as native- vs. foreign-accented speakers). We explore how children reconcile these 2 strategies when they are put in conflict and whether…
Descriptors: Young Children, English, Native Speakers, Dialects
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sprung, Manuel; Harris, Paul L. – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2010
Background: "Hurricane Katrina" devastated the Mississippi Gulf Coast in August 2005. Intrusive re-experiencing is a common posttraumatic stress symptom. However, young children with limited introspection skills might have difficulties identifying their intrusive thoughts. Method: A sample of 165 5- to 9-year-old children were surveyed about their…
Descriptors: Natural Disasters, Child Abuse, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bascandziev, Igor; Harris, Paul L. – Cognitive Development, 2010
Previous research has shown that young children make a perseverative, gravity-oriented, error when asked to predict the final location of a ball dropped down an S-shaped opaque tube (Hood, 1995). We asked if providing children with verbal information concerning the role that the tubes play, in determining the ball's trajectory would improve their…
Descriptors: Cues, Young Children, Internet, Physics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Corriveau, Kathleen; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Science, 2009
In two experiments, children aged 3, 4 and 5 years (N = 61) were given conflicting information about the names and functions of novel objects by two informants, one a familiar teacher, the other an unfamiliar teacher. On pre-test trials, all three age groups invested more trust in the familiar teacher. They preferred to ask for information and to…
Descriptors: Trust (Psychology), Familiarity, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Kim, Angie L.; Schwalen, Courtney E.; Harris, Paul L. – Cognition, 2009
Based on the testimony of others, children learn about a variety of figures that they never meet. We ask when and how they are able to differentiate between the historical figures that they learn about (e.g., Abraham Lincoln) and fantasy characters (e.g., Harry Potter). Experiment 1 showed that both younger (3- and 4-year-olds) and older children…
Descriptors: Fantasy, History, Young Children, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Harris, Paul L.; Meins, Elizabeth; Fernyhough, Charles; Arnott, Bronia; Elliott, Lorna; Liddle, Beth; Hearn, Alexandra; Vittorini, Lucia; de Rosnay, Marc – Child Development, 2009
In a longitudinal study of attachment, children (N = 147) aged 50 and 61 months heard their mother and a stranger make conflicting claims. In 2 tasks, the available perceptual cues were equally consistent with either person's claim but children generally accepted the mother's claims over those of the stranger. In a 3rd task, the perceptual cues…
Descriptors: Cues, Mothers, Attachment Behavior, Trust (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fusaro, Maria; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Science, 2008
Recent findings show that preschool children are selective with respect to whom they ask for information and whose claims they endorse. In particular, they monitor an informant's record of past accuracy or inaccuracy and use that record to gauge future trustworthiness. We ask if preschoolers also monitor the non-verbal cues of assent or dissent…
Descriptors: Cues, Prior Learning, Interpersonal Communication, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Science, 2007
Children rely extensively on others' testimony to learn about the world. However, they are not uniformly credulous toward other people. From an early age, children's reliance on testimony is tempered by selective trust in particular informants. Three- and 4-year-olds monitor the accuracy or knowledge of informants, including those that are…
Descriptors: Trust (Psychology), Young Children, Developmental Stages, Interpersonal Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Harris, Paul L.; Pasquini, Elisabeth S.; Duke, Suzanne; Asscher, Jessica J.; Pons, Francisco – Developmental Science, 2006
In three experiments, children's reliance on other people's testimony as compared to their own, first-hand experience was assessed in the domain of ontology. Children ranging from 4 to 8 years were asked to judge whether five different types of entity exist: real entities (e.g. cats, trees) whose existence is evident to everyone; scientific…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Experiments, Young Children, Fairy Tales
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Harris, Paul L.; And Others – Cognitive Development, 1997
Two experiments tested children's ability to imagine a pretend action and select a representation of its outcome. Found that children two years and older could select the correct representation, whether represented by a picture or toy; younger children could not select representations of actual or pretend transformations. Results had implications…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Imagination
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Harris, Paul L.; And Others – Child Development, 1985
Western and Chinese children six years of age judged that an initially intense positive or negative emotional reaction would wane gradually over time. Children four years of age were less consistent, but, when steps were taken to insure their comprehension, they too judged that emotion wanes gradually over time. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Comparative Analysis, Comprehension, Emotional Experience
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3