Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Nonverbal Communication | 2 |
Pretests Posttests | 2 |
Young Children | 2 |
Children | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
Experiments | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Instructional Effectiveness | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Memory | 1 |
Models | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Goldin-Meadow, Susan | 2 |
Factor, Laiah | 1 |
Hemani, Naureen | 1 |
Levine, Susan C. | 1 |
Ping, Raedy M. | 1 |
Yip, Terina KuangYi | 1 |
Zinchenko, Elena | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Audience
Location
Illinois | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Levine, Susan C.; Zinchenko, Elena; Yip, Terina KuangYi; Hemani, Naureen; Factor, Laiah – Developmental Science, 2012
Performing action has been found to have a greater impact on learning than observing action. Here we ask whether a particular type of action--the gestures that accompany talk--affect learning in a comparable way. We gave 158 6-year-old children instruction in a mental transformation task. Half the children were asked to produce a "Move"…
Descriptors: Young Children, Nonverbal Communication, Comparative Analysis, Pretests Posttests
Ping, Raedy M.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Developmental Psychology, 2008
Including gesture in instruction facilitates learning. Why? One possibility is that gesture points out objects in the immediate context and thus helps ground the words learners hear in the world they see. Previous work on gesture's role in instruction has used gestures that either point to or trace paths on objects, thus providing support for this…
Descriptors: Symbolic Language, Nonverbal Communication, Pretests Posttests, Models