Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Adults | 9 |
Young Children | 9 |
Age Differences | 5 |
Classification | 5 |
Cognitive Development | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Concept Formation | 3 |
Developmental Stages | 3 |
Inferences | 3 |
Semantics | 3 |
Animals | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 2 |
Cognition | 2 |
Developmental Science | 1 |
International Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Child Language | 1 |
Journal of Cognition and… | 1 |
Language and Cognitive… | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Education Level
Preschool Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Michigan | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Marchak, Kristan A.; Bayly, Bryana; Umscheid, Valerie; Gelman, Susan A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2020
When reasoning about a representation (e.g., a toy lion), children often engage in "iconic realism," whereby representations are reported to have properties of their real-life referents. The present studies examined an inverse difficulty that we dub "representational disregard": overlooking (i.e., disregarding) a…
Descriptors: Young Children, Adults, Age Differences, Logical Thinking
Hollander, Michelle A.; Gelman, Susan A.; Raman, Lakshmi – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2009
Many languages distinguish generic utterances (e.g., "Tigers are ferocious") from non-generic utterances (e.g., "Those tigers are ferocious"). Two studies examined how generic language specially links properties and categories. We used a novel-word extension task to ask if 4- to 5-year-old children and adults distinguish…
Descriptors: Semantics, Animals, Adults, Young Children
Rhodes, Marjorie; Gelman, Susan A.; Brickman, Daniel – Developmental Science, 2010
Two studies compared children's attention to sample composition--whether a sample provides a diverse representation of a category of interest--during teacher-led and learner-driven learning contexts. In Study 1 (n = 48), 5-year-olds attended to sample composition to make inferences about biological properties only when samples were presented by a…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Inferences, Writing (Composition), Teaching Methods
Taylor, Marianne G.; Rhodes, Marjorie; Gelman, Susan A. – Child Development, 2009
Two studies (N = 456) compared the development of concepts of animal species and human gender, using a switched-at-birth reasoning task. Younger children (5- and 6-year-olds) treated animal species and human gender as equivalent; they made similar levels of category-based inferences and endorsed similar explanations for development in these 2…
Descriptors: Animals, Classification, Environmental Influences, Inferences
Bares, Cristina B.; Gelman, Susan A. – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2008
Research on children's knowledge of illnesses has largely concentrated on studying how children reason about common innocuous diseases. It is also important to uncover how children reason about more severe diseases, such as cancer, to be able to treat and communicate with children diagnosed with this disease. Several aspects of prevalent childhood…
Descriptors: Cancer, Young Children, Intuition, Diseases
Gelman, Susan A.; Bloom, Paul – Cognition, 2007
Generic sentences (such as "Birds lay eggs") are important in that they refer to kinds (e.g., birds as a group) rather than individuals (e.g., the birds in the henhouse). The present set of studies examined aspects of how generic nouns are understood by English speakers. Adults and children (4- and 5-year-olds) were presented with scenarios about…
Descriptors: Semantics, Sentences, Nouns, Cognitive Processes
Jipson, Jennifer L.; Gelman, Susan A. – Child Development, 2007
This study tests the firm distinction children are said to make between living and nonliving kinds. Three, 4-, and 5-year-old children and adults reasoned about whether items that varied on 3 dimensions (alive, face, behavior) had a range of properties (biological, psychological, perceptual, artifact, novel, proper names). Findings demonstrate…
Descriptors: Inferences, Differences, Young Children, Adults

Ahn, Woo-kyoung; Gelman, Susan A.; Amsterlaw, Jennifer A.; Hohenstein, Jill; Kalish, Charles W. – Cognition, 2000
Examined causal status effect (weighing cause features more than effect features in categorization). Presented adults and 7- to 9-year-olds animal descriptions wherein one feature caused two others. Asked which transfer item was more likely an example of novel animal. Found that both groups preferred an animal with a cause and an effect feature…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development

Gelman, Susan A.; Koenig, Melissa A. – Journal of Child Language, 2001
Examines whether children make use of the conceptual link between animacy and agency when interpreting the verb "move" in English. Hypothesized that, for inanimates, children would allow "move" to have a patient subject but not so for inanimates. Subjects were 3- and 4-year-olds and adults who viewed video clips of animals or…
Descriptors: Adults, Child Language, English (Second Language), Language Acquisition