Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Memory | 2 |
Older Adults | 2 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Associative Learning | 1 |
Cognitive Ability | 1 |
Cognitive Tests | 1 |
Color | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
Conflict | 1 |
Error Patterns | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Antonio García Marín | 1 |
Borja Montejo Rubio | 1 |
Bugg, Julie M. | 1 |
David Prada Crespo | 1 |
M. Emiliana De Andrés Montes | 1 |
Mercedes Montenegro-Peña | 1 |
Pedro Montejo Carrasco | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 2 |
Trail Making Test | 1 |
Wechsler Memory Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Mercedes Montenegro-Peña; Pedro Montejo Carrasco; M. Emiliana De Andrés Montes; Antonio García Marín; Borja Montejo Rubio; David Prada Crespo – Educational Gerontology, 2025
The results of Cognitive Intervention (CI) programs vary considerably; thus, it is helpful to assess the characteristics that enhance the effectiveness of these programs. Our objectives were to analyze the effectiveness of a multicomponent CI program called UMAM on cognitive performance, subjective memory, daily forgetfulness, and mood of…
Descriptors: Memory, Intervention, Program Effectiveness, Older Adults
Bugg, Julie M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
The conflict monitoring account posits that globally high levels of conflict trigger engagement of top-down control; however, recent findings point to the mercurial nature of top-down control in high conflict contexts. The current study examined the potential moderating effect of associative learning on conflict-triggered top-down control…
Descriptors: Conflict, Experimental Psychology, Associative Learning, Hypothesis Testing