Original research

Visual processing capacity and cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease

Abstract

Background Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder characterised by motor symptoms. However, approximately half of patients with PD exhibit signs of dementia within a decade of diagnosis. While deficits in working memory and visuospatial abilities are recognised as hallmarks of cognitive decline in PD, these populations are rarely studied using detailed cognitive tools that link cognitive impairments to formal theoretical models, such as the theory of visual attention (TVA).

Methods This cross-sectional study addresses this gap by employing the TVA whole report paradigm to assess visual processing in a cohort of patients with PD, both with and without cognitive impairment. Participants were divided based on their Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) scores into two PD groups (n=25 each) and a healthy control group (n=25).

Results Our principal finding is that the visual processing speed (C) and visual short-term memory capacity (K) are significantly diminished in patients with PD with MoCA scores below 26 (Analysis of variance, p=0.016 for C and p<0.001 for K), while no notable differences were observed between controls and patients with PD with MoCA scores of 26 or above. Using a generalised linear model to assess the impact of factors such as age, gender and disease duration, we discovered that the C-parameter was significantly influenced by age, while the K-parameter was notably affected by gender.

Conclusion TVA parameters demonstrate their suitability for detecting cognitive deficits in PD. Given their independence from motor and non-motor symptoms, TVA parameters may prove to be valuable tools for early diagnosis and longitudinal monitoring of cognitive deficits in individual patients with PD.

What is already known on this topic

  • The theory of visual attention (TVA) is well established in other neurodegenerative disorders and offers certain advantages for cognitive assessment in Parkinson’s disease. These include independence from motor impairment and the targeting of visuospatial function as a key indicator of cognitive decline.

What this study adds

  • This study demonstrates that the TVA parameters can detect cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Patients with cognitive impairment showed significantly reduced visual processing speed C and diminished visual short-term memory capacity K compared with both cognitively unimpaired patients and healthy controls. Furthermore, the correlation of TVA parameters with clinical parameters underscores their potential value in understanding and assessing cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease.

How this study might affect research, practice or policy

  • TVA-based assessment may serve as a valid tool for detecting and monitoring cognitive deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease.

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disease, characterised by motor symptoms such as bradykinesia, tremor and rigidity. Partly, PD is caused by the loss of dopamine-producing neurons in the midbrain, resulting in decreased dopamine in the basal ganglia, a region of the brain deeply connected to the prefrontal cortex.1 Consequently, in addition to motor impairments, cognitive deficits are common in PD and tend to worsen over time. Longitudinal studies have shown that 10 years after PD diagnosis, dementia prevails in around 46% of patients.2

Cognitive impairment in patients with PD varies and can comprise heterogeneous symptom clusters. According to the ‘dual syndrome hypothesis’,3 deficits in executive function and working memory are related to dopaminergic depletion within frontal-striatal networks, whereas impairments in anterograde memory and visuospatial functions are related to cholinergic deficiencies in posterior cortical regions and the temporal lobes. While the ‘frontal-striatal’ deficits seem to remain relatively stable during the course of PD, the ‘posterior’ deficits are associated with a more rapid cognitive decline and the development of dementia.3 This has also been shown in more recent longitudinal studies.4 5 Therefore, the posterior symptom cluster seems to bear predictive value for estimating the risk of dementia.

From a neuropsychological perspective, deficits of visuospatial perception are the hallmark of posterior cortical dysfunction.3 5 Thus, it is not surprising that tasks tapping this cognitive domain have shown great potential for predicting cognitive decline. For example, tests of lower level visual function, such as contrast sensitivity and visual acuity, have shown that deficits on these tests are strongly associated with cognitive performance in PD.6 Furthermore, Weil et al found that despite having similar cognitive scores, patients with PD performed worse than healthy controls (HC) on a task of identifying briefly presented cat and dog images.7 Poorer performance on this task has been found in patients with PD at high risk for dementia8 and poor visual performance was linked to cognitive decline and white matter damage.9 While the sensitivity of visual tasks for evaluating cognitive decline or dementia risk in PD is well established, the underlying mechanisms hampering visual processing in PD are incompletely understood.

The theory of visual attention (TVA) offers the possibility of separately assessing the underlying components of visual processing capacity within the same task (for a more detailed theoretical background, see the online supplemental material). The mathematical TVA model provides parameter estimates for visual processing speed and visual short-term memory (VSTM) capacity.10 The TVA-based assessment of these parameters is largely independent of motor status, making it particularly suited for patients with PD where motor impairments can confound cognitive assessments. In previous studies, we have successfully applied this method in early Alzheimer’s disease11 12 and in patients with Huntington’s disease, where TVA-based analyses showed associations of TVA parameters with the degree of the genetic defect13 and with impaired simultaneous perception of visual objects.14 Thus, future studies can build on this approach and enhance our understanding by relating TVA parameters to specific neuropathological and behavioural aspects of PD.

In the current study, our aim was to assess the efficiency of visual information uptake in patients with PD based on the application of the TVA. Given the known relationship between short-term maintenance of information with the dopaminergic system,15 and between visual processing speed and the cholinergic system,16 we hypothesised that both TVA parameters reflecting these cognitive components would be reduced in patients with PD with cognitive impairment compared with those without cognitive impairment and the HC. As a secondary objective, we assessed the association of TVA-derived parameters with clinical features relevant to PD, such as age, disease duration, gender and disease severity. We aimed to show how TVA parameters relate to PD progression, providing valuable insight into their clinical significance.

Methods

Subjects

In this study, 75 participants were divided into three different groups based on their Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) scores (table 1): 25 PD-ND (patients with PD with no decline indicated by a MoCA score ≥26; 14 female; age: M=62.5, SD=7.28), 25 PD-CD (patients with PD with cognitive decline indicated by a MoCA score ≤25; 16 female; age: M=66.8, SD=6.99), and 25 HC (14 female; age M=62.7, SD=7.14). We recruited all patients with PD who were undergoing treatment in the Center for Movement Disorders at the Jena University Hospital. HC were recruited via information in local media. Inclusion and exclusion criteria for the study were as follows: all patients with PD were required to have clinically established PD (according to the 2015 Movement Disorder Society (MDS) criteria17), and either normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants with clinically relevant symptoms of depression, as indicated by the depression subscore of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS-D) above the cut-off (>10),18 a prior diagnosis of dementia or other relevant neurological diseases were excluded.

Table 1
Demographic and neuropsychological data for the three subject groups taking part in the study

To outline the groups’ characteristics and account for potential confounders, we collected relevant clinical parameters: motor disability and disease stage were assessed using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) part III19 and the Hoehn and Yahr scale.20 To screen for non-motor symptoms in PD, the non-motor Symptoms Questionnaire21 and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)18 were employed. Additionally, we assessed impulse control disorders with the Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in PD Rating Scale (QUIP-RS)22 and the quality of life with the Parkinson’s disease Questionnaire 8 (PDQ-8).23 We documented the levodopa equivalent dosage and the disease duration.

To achieve equal group sizes, we initially recruited 60 patients with PD, targeting two groups of 30 patients each based on the MoCA cut-off (PD-ND: MoCA ≥26 and PD-CD: MoCA ≤25). After recruitment, we corrected for demographic and clinical factors such as age, gender and disease duration, forming matched pairs of patients in each PD group. HC were subsequently included from a precollected dataset, matched on demographic factors, resulting in three equally sized groups.

24

Patient and public involvement

Patients were directly involved in this study as subjects. All received detailed information and consent forms to ensure they understood the aims and procedures of the study and their rights. We spoke to patients and ward staff to explain the study and assess the ability to participate. No members of the public were involved in this study.

Whole report

We conducted the TVA-based whole report paradigm described in detail by Martin et al.25 The experiment was conducted in a dimly lit room on a 100 Hz computer screen and a viewing distance of 50 cm. It took approximately 20 min to complete and consisted of four practice blocks with 48 trials and four test blocks with 84 trials. Each trial had the same basic design (figure 1). A fixation point in the centre of the black screen initiated the trial and appeared for 1000 ms. Following the fixation point with a delay of 250 ms, six stimuli (red or blue capital letters) appeared on a black background. All stimuli were randomly selected from the alphabet (except for I, Q and Y) and each letter was displayed at most once per trial. The stimuli were arranged in a circle equidistantly at a visual angle of 5.73° around the fixation point. Five different stimulus exposure durations were used across trials. For each participant, these five exposure durations were individually determined, based on the performance in four practice blocks. The aim was to obtain data from the full range of performance, from levels below and close to threshold and at maximum, in order to optimise the fitting procedure’s validity. Depending on the trial condition, the stimuli were presented either masked (in five conditions) or unmasked (in two conditions). Masks appeared for 500 ms and were used to better control the presentation time by deleting the visual afterimage.26 The participant’s task was to report as many letters as possible but refrain from guessing. The experimenter recorded the answers.

Whole report paradigm: example for one trial. The fixation point initiates the trial, followed by a stimulus display. In the whole report, the stimulus at display consists of six letters and all of them are either red or blue. After the stimulus, in most trials, a pattern mask is presented. The only exceptions to this are two trial conditions where no masking is applied. Finally, the participant reports the recognised stimuli verbally. Figure created with Biorender.com

Based on this performance, the TVA parameters processing speed C and VSTM capacity K were derived. The parameters were fitted and generated in MATLAB (V.9.13.0, R2020a) based on a maximum-likelihood procedure.27 The relationship between performance accuracy and presentation time is graphically represented by an exponential growth function.28 Whole report parameters were extracted from this function: The VSTM capacity K is the asymptote of the growth function standing for the number of stimuli that can be simultaneously processed and processing speed C is illustrated by the initial slope of the function representing the rate of stimuli processed per second.28

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

The presence of cognitive impairment was assessed by the MoCA,29 a standard screening test validated to distinguish healthy subjects from patients with mild cognitive impairment and recommended by the MDS PD-MCI Task Force as an economic measure for diagnosing mild cognitive impairment in patients with PD.30 The MoCA was applied by examiners trained according to the standard guidelines (see www.mocacognition.com). In accordance with the MDS Task Force recommendations, a score <26 was considered as indicating cognitive impairment, while scores ≥26 were considered as normal (maximum score: 30).

Statistical analysis

Data analysis was performed using the Konstanz Information Miner (KNIME: V.5.1.2).31 All groups were matched for age, gender and years of education. Participants who could not be matched or who had missing data were excluded. To identify group differences in both clinical characteristics and TVA parameters, we compared the three groups using an analysis of variance. To explore differences between the PD groups, pairwise comparisons using t-tests were performed. Levene’s test for equality of variances was applied to ensure homogeneity of variance across these comparisons.

Associations between TVA parameters and clinical characteristics were analysed using Pearson correlation. To identify potential clinical confounders affecting the TVA parameters, a generalised linear model (GLM) was performed.

Finally, to assess the predictive power of TVA parameters in distinguishing between PD-ND and PD-CD, a logistic regression classifier was employed to determine whether TVA parameters could effectively predict the grouping of patients with PD based on their cognitive status as indicated by the MoCA scores.

Results were considered significant if p≤0.05. A more detailed description of the statistical methods is provided in the online supplemental material.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants

The demographic and clinical characteristics of the groups are summarised in table 1. Groups did not differ in terms of gender distribution (F=2.36, p=0.103), education (F=0.63, p=0.538) and age (F=2.65, p=0.078). MoCA scores differed between the groups (F=66.94, p<0.001): while HC (M=28.5, SD=1.62) and PD-ND (M=27.6, SD=1.60) did not differ from each other, PC-CD (M=21.3, SD=2.10) had lower scores compared with both HC (T=14.57, p<0.001) and PD-ND (T=9.36, p<0.001). The PD groups differed in some clinical aspects, with PD-ND having a milder disease stage (T=−2.80, p=0.008) and lower depression scores (F=8.09, p=0.001). There were no further statistical differences in the other collected clinical parameters.

TVA parameters and comparative group analysis

Our analysis of the TVA parameters revealed notable differences in visual processing between the groups (table 2 and figure 2). In summary, main effects for the whole report parameters indicated lower visual processing speed C and VSTM capacity K in PD-CD compared with HC and PD-ND. Post hoc t-tests revealed no significant difference between HC (M=27.46, SD=9.53) and PD-ND (M=27.76, SD=11.62) in the C-parameter (T=−0.9; p=0.925). However, the C-parameter was significantly lower in PD-CD (M=20.45, SD=10.64) compared with the HC and PD-ND (T=2.22; p=0.031 for PD-ND vs PD-CD and T=2.35; p=0.023 for HC vs PD-CD). Similarly, VSTM capacity K did not differ between HC (M=2.85, SD=0.67) and PD-ND (M=2.88, SD=0.64) (T=−0.15; p=0.880). However, PD-CD exhibited a significantly lower K-parameter (M=2.12, SD=0.5), compared with both HC (T=4.22; p<0.001) and PD-ND (T=4.50; p<0.001).

Boxplots of fitted TVA parameters. Groups: HC, healthy controls; PD-ND, patients with PD with MoCA≥26; PD-CD, patients with PD with MoCA≤25. *Significant group differences in t-test for independent groups, differences are considered significant a p≤0.05. MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; TVA, theory of visual attention; VSTM, visual short-term memory.

Table 2
Fitted TVA parameters

The effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for the reduction in C in the PD-CD group were 0.69 (PD-CD vs HC) and 0.66 (PD-CD vs PD-ND), indicating moderate effects. For K, the effect sizes were 1.24 (PD-CD vs HC) and 1.33 (PD-CD vs PD-ND), indicating large effects. These values underscore the relatively greater deviation observed for the K-parameter.

TVA parameters and demographic and clinical features

We then analysed the relationship between various demographic and clinical parameters and TVA parameters in all patients with PD (n=50) without distinguishing between PD-ND and PD-CD using pairwise Pearson’s correlation analyses and GLM.

Correlation analysis

The correlation analysis (figure 3 and online supplemental table S1) revealed significant correlations (p≤0.05, false discovery rate corrected) that link demographic and clinical features to TVA parameters. Significant negative correlations were found between age and the C-parameter (r=−0.59, p<0.001), with higher age being related to lower processing speed in patients with PD. Furthermore, the C (r=0.33, p=0.021) and K (r=0.37, p=0.008) parameters showed positive correlations with MoCA scores, indicating that better cognitive function is associated with better processing speed and VSTM capacity.

Correlation matrix of TVA parameters and PD-specific clinical scores (N = 50). Pairwise correlations (Pearson’s correlation) between clinical markers, scores and TVA parameters, with non-significant correlations marked by an X. Values are considered significant at p≤0.05, after correction for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (FDR) method. The intensity of the correlation coefficient is depicted by colour, with red for 1 and blue for −1. MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; PD, Parkinson’s disease; QUIP-RS, Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in PD Rating Scale; NMS-Quest, non-motor Symptoms Questionnaire; PDQ-8, Parkinson’s disease Questionnaire 8; TVA, theory of visual attention; UPDRS, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; VSTM, visual short-term memory.

The Hoehn and Yahr scale was moderately correlated with MoCA scores (r=−0.55, p<0.001) and the C-parameter (r=−0.33, p=0.017). This implies that as the clinical severity of PD increases, the general cognitive status and visual processing speed decrease.

While there was a negative but non-significant correlation between UPDRS III and MoCA scores (r=−0.25, p=0.080), there was almost no correlation between UPDRS III and the TVA parameters (for C, r=0.09, p=0.534; for K, r=−0.15, p=0.289).

Both disease duration (r=−0.30, p=0.035) and levodopa equivalent dosage (r=−0.33, p=0.018) were negatively correlated with the C-parameter, which demonstrated that slower processing speed was associated with longer disease duration and higher medication.

Furthermore, the MoCA was negatively correlated with PDQ-8 (r=−0.40, p=0.004), QUIP-RS (r=−0.35, p=0.013), HADS-A (r=−0.43, p=0.002) and HADS-D (r=−0.33, p=0.020). The TVA parameters only showed a negative correlation between the K-parameter and HADS-D (r=−0.29, p=0.045), indicating lower VSTM capacity in patients with higher depression scores.

The significant linear relationships are further illustrated in scatter plots provided in the online supplemental material (online supplemental figures S1 and S2). These plots depict the relationships between age and visual processing speed C and gender and VSTM capacity K, with individual data points colour-coded to differentiate between groups.

Generalised linear model

The GLM analysis included TVA parameters C and K as dependent variables (table 3). The first model incorporated age, gender, disease duration and levodopa equivalent dosage as independent variables. An extended GLM model, presented in the online supplemental material (online supplemental table S2), also included the grouping variable (0=PD ND, 1=PD CD) and the interaction between group and age.

Table 3
GLM analysis results for TVA parameters and various clinical factors
Visual processing speed (C)

Age and levodopa equivalent dosage were found to have main effects on the C-parameter. In the extended model, the grouping variable did not significantly predict C, indicating that age was the predominant factor influencing processing speed, irrespective of cognitive status (PD-ND vs PD-CD).

VSTM capacity (K)

Gender showed a significant main effect on the K-parameter (coefficient=0.69 indicating higher values in women), with an interaction between age and group also being significant. This suggests that storage capacity in VSTM is influenced by gender and is differently impacted by age in PD-ND and PD-CD.

MoCA score

PD-ND and PD-CD were primarily separated based on their MoCA scores and there was a slight but significant difference regarding their disease stage with PD-CD being slightly more advanced (table 1). The GLM analysis demonstrated a strong effect of the disease stage20 on the dependent variable MoCA (coefficient =−4.42, z-score=−2.74). However, disease duration, gender and age did not significantly influence the MoCA scores.

Depression subscore of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS-D)

As there was a significant difference in depression scores in both PD groups, we performed further GLM analysis including this factor, but it was not significant and did not change the significance of the other main effects.

Education

To address the factor of education, we extended the GLM model with the binary variable for education (0–12 years or less, 1—more than 12 years of education); here education did not significantly predict processing speed C (p=0.135), VSTM capacity K (p=0.553) or MoCA scores (p=0.391).

Regression model for grouping PD-ND and PD-CD with TVA parameters and their influencing factors age and gender

To examine the interactions between the TVA parameters C and K and their interaction effects from the GLM (with grouping) and to identify which parameters can predict group membership (PD-ND vs PD-CD), we employed a logistic regression model. The target variable was group membership and the independent variables were visual processing speed C, VSTM capacity K, age, gender and HADS-D. The model revealed a significant effect for K (coefficient=3.14, z=2.83, p=0.005), while C was not significant (coefficient=0.06, z=1.54, p=0.124). The additional covariates, age (coefficient=−0.03, z=−0.48, p=0.631) and gender (coefficient=0.64, z=0.66, p=0.509), and HADS-D (coefficient=−0.20, z=−1.65 p=0.099) were also not significant.

Discussion

Summary of key findings

It is well established that visual tasks bear a great potential for evaluating the dementia risk in PD. Object discrimination tasks using brief stimulus presentations and lower level visual function tests assessing visual processing speed and visual memory have been proven as appropriate measures in this regard.6–9

Therefore, in this study, we investigated the efficiency of visual information uptake in patients with PD using the TVA-based assessment. Our main findings indicate that patients with PD with cognitive deficits exhibit significantly diminished visual processing speed C and VSTM capacity K. These deficits were not observed in patients with PD without cognitive impairment when compared with HC. Additionally, our correlation and GLM analyses revealed that TVA parameters were significantly associated with demographic and clinical factors such as age, gender, disease severity and medication dosage in patients with PD. These results suggest that the TVA-based assessment can effectively identify early cognitive deficits in PD and may serve as a valuable tool for tracking cognitive decline.

Our findings highlight the added value of TVA-based analysis for the assessment of cognitive decline in PD. This approach provides highly sensitive and specific quantitative parameters (C and K), allowing for a more nuanced characterisation of attentional deficits compared with standard cognitive tests. TVA’s robust theoretical framework enhances diagnostic accuracy by linking cognitive impairments to distinct attentional mechanisms, which is particularly important given the heterogeneity of cognitive deficits in PD. In addition, its non-paced performance design mitigates the confounding effects of motor impairments, ensuring more reliable cognitive assessments in this population.

Visual processing speed (C)

The C-parameter is crucial for understanding the efficiency with which patients with PD process visual information. The parameters’ observed impairment among patients with PD-CD suggests a reduced ability to quickly and effectively process visual stimuli, which could be linked to dopaminergic deficits in the basal ganglia and its connections with the prefrontal cortex.1 This finding is consistent with the pathology of PD, where dopaminergic neuron degeneration leads to motor and cognitive impairment. Furthermore, it has been suggested that a cholinergic deficit might lead to impaired processing of visual stimuli.3 11

Our findings from the extended GLM analysis indicate that age is the primary predictor of processing speed C and appears to have a stronger influence than cognitive status (PD-ND vs PD-CD). This highlights the significant impact of ageing on visual processing efficiency, potentially overshadowing group-specific effects related to cognitive decline observed in this study.

Additionally, functional connectivity within the cingulo-opercular network, which includes fronto-insular regions, the thalamus and basal ganglia, has been linked to visual processing speed deficits.32 Disruptions in this network, as seen in PD, may further exacerbate impairments in attentional control and visual information uptake.

VSTM capacity (K)

Similarly, the K-parameter showed an impairment in patients with PD-CD, potentially associated with deficits in the dorsal attention network, which is involved in the allocation of attentional resources and working memory processing.33 The diminished capacity could result from disruptions in the network due to neurodegenerative changes in PD.

Recent evidence has linked VSTM capacity to structural integrity within key white matter tracts. Chechlacz et al showed that individual differences in VSTM are associated with the microstructure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (II, III) and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus in the right hemisphere.34 These tracts are critical for integrating visual information and maintaining attentional focus, both of which are essential for VSTM capacity. In PD, Chen et al further established an association between the superior longitudinal fasciculus and global cognitive performance as measured by the MoCA, suggesting that degeneration of these tracts contributes to broader cognitive deficits.35

The pronounced reduction in VSTM capacity K in patients with PD-CD compared with the other groups suggests that this parameter may be more sensitive to neurodegenerative changes in PD than visual processing speed C. This is consistent with the role of the dorsal attention network, which is integral to VSTM capacity and may be particularly vulnerable in PD. The larger effect size for K compared with C highlights the potential clinical utility of K as a marker of cognitive impairment in PD.

Clinical cofactors

The strong correlations between the TVA parameters and clinical features such as age, gender and disease severity highlight the multifaceted nature of cognitive decline in PD. These findings align with previously described predictors for cognitive decline in PD.2

To be clear, age and gender are primarily demographic variables, but in the context of neurodegenerative diseases such as PD, they also influence clinical features such as disease progression and cognitive reserve. For example, the negative correlation between age and both C and K-parameters highlights the progressive nature of cognitive decline in ageing patients with PD. Similar age-related declines in these parameters have also been reported in healthy individuals.36 Nielsen and colleagues provided a more nuanced perspective by demonstrating that processing speed C is more affected by age than VSTM capacity K in a cohort of 112 healthy adults aged 60—75 years.37 This is consistent with our finding of a strong effect of age on C, suggesting that changes in processing speed may primarily reflect age-related mechanisms. In contrast, K appears to be more sensitive to cognitive decline due to neurodegeneration. We propose that changes in C are largely driven by ageing processes, whereas K reflects cognitive decline associated with progressive neurodegeneration of structures critical for VSTM capacity. This distinction highlights the potential of TVA parameters to discriminate between age-related changes and disease-specific cognitive deficits.

Furthermore, the gender differences observed in the K-parameter in the PD groups may indicate differences in cognitive reserve or disease progression between male and female patients with PD. In our study, female participants had higher K-parameters than their male counterparts, suggesting a more robust functioning of VSTM. Interestingly, the existing literature has not found gender differences in TVA parameters in healthy older adults.36 37 Thus, our finding seems to be PD-specific and fits to the fact that while the age-adjusted prevalence of dementia is higher in women in the general population,38 cognitive deficits associated with PD are more common in men.2 We acknowledge that the observed gender-related effect on the K-parameter has not been previously reported in TVA-based studies and may reflect either a true sex-specific influence in PD or a sample-specific finding. For example, Pourzinal et al reported on different mild cognitive impairment subtypes in PD, with male patients being over-represented in the globally impaired subtype, and females belonging more frequently to the cognitively intact subtype.39 Future studies with larger and more diverse cohorts are needed to replicate and further explore this result.

Education, as included in our analysis as a binary factor (≤12 years or >12 years), did not significantly influence TVA parameters. However, its potential role in shaping cognitive reserve warrants further investigation.

Disease progression, as measured by the Hoehn and Yahr scale, has been identified as a primary risk factor for the emergence of cognitive deficits in PD.2 Consistent with this observation, our data reveal a detrimental effect of disease progression on both the C-parameter and MoCA scores, underscoring the negative impact of advancing PD on cognitive functions. In comparison to other neurodegenerative diseases, our results align with findings from studies on Huntington’s disease13 and Alzheimer’s disease.11 28 In Huntington’s disease, significant reductions in both C and K-parameters have been reported, with a strong correlation to disease duration. Similarly, in Alzheimer’s disease and its precursor, mild cognitive impairment, evidence of a staged decline has been obtained. While C and K-parameters are slightly decreased in mild cognitive impairment, they exhibit a marked decline as the disease progresses to manifest Alzheimer’s disease.11

Non-motor symptoms such as depression, anxiety, fatigue and reduced impulse control are often found in patients with PD and highly affect patients’ quality of life. Furthermore, they are often connected to more pronounced cognitive deficits.40 We found that higher scores in these measures were significantly associated with lower MoCA scores. In contrast, TVA parameters were largely independent of these non-motor symptoms. While there was a slight correlation between depressive symptoms and the K-parameter, the C-parameter was unaffected. This suggests that TVA-based assessments provide a reliable measure of cognitive function in PD that is not confounded by non-motor symptoms.

Clinical implications

As there was no significant difference between HC and PD-ND, but a clear difference between PD-CD and the other two groups, the TVA-based assessment of attentional functions may serve as a valid tool for the early detection of cognitive deficits in PD. Due to its objective nature and minimal habituation effects,10 the TVA-based assessment could be instrumental in monitoring cognitive decline as the disease progresses. The TVA parameters are mathematically independent measures of separable, relevant attentional components derived from performance in a single task. The lack of correlation between C and K in this study underscores their independence and ability to differentiate distinct facets of cognition. Unlike typical attentional tasks, the TVA-based assessment does not require speeded motor responses, but only non-speeded vocal responses, making this methodology well-suited for patients with severe motor problems.

Additionally, TVA-based parameters may provide a robust framework for tracking longitudinal changes in cognitive decline, offering insights into the trajectory of disease progression and potential treatment effects.

Furthermore, the TVA-based assessment may be effective in evaluating the impact of treatment strategies. For example, cholinergic stimulation has shown promise in enhancing certain TVA parameters in Alzheimer’s disease patients,11 suggesting potential applicability in PD treatment evaluations.

Limitations

This study has limitations. Despite efforts to balance the clinical cofactors such as age, gender, and disease duration, the PD-CD group was slightly more advanced clinically, as evidenced by higher scores on the Hoehn and Yahr scale.

Participants were selected based on having no diagnosis of dementia, and grouping was based on MoCA scores (cut-off: 26). Using a single cut-off for cognitive impairment may not fully account for potentially confounding factors such as age and education. However, we controlled for these factors by matching groups for age and education, and by using a MoCA cut-off that includes an extra point for education under 12 years, minimising their potential impact on our results. Future studies might benefit from more comprehensive neuropsychological assessments for grouping participants.

The current HC group is a valid neurotypical comparison. Future studies must include a separate, larger group of neurotypical older adults. This is essential to clarify the influence of age on TVA parameters independent of PD-related pathology. This will enhance the statistical power for detecting age-specific versus disease-specific effects on visual processing speed C and VSTM capacity K.

Additionally, future studies might benefit from more comprehensive neuropsychological assessments for grouping participants. Longitudinal studies would also allow insights into how TVA parameters change with disease progression and may help to predict the trajectory of cognitive impairment.

In conclusion, this study contributes significantly to the understanding of cognitive decline in PD through the innovative application of the TVA. We demonstrated that TVA parameters processing speed (C) and VSTM capacity (K) are significantly impaired in patients with PD with early cognitive deficits. These findings highlight the potential of TVA-based assessment methodology as a sensitive tool for detecting and monitoring cognitive impairment in PD.

Importantly, our results suggest that cognitive decline in PD is closely linked to deficits in visual processing capacity, which can be quantitatively assessed using TVA. Critical merits of the TVA-based assessment lie in its ability to measure cognitive deficits independently of motor and non-motor symptoms, making it especially well-suited for patients with PD.

TVA-based assessment shows promise as a valuable tool for early diagnosis and longitudinal monitoring of cognitive deficits in PD. By incorporating TVA into clinical practice, we may enhance early detection and monitor disease progression more effectively.

  • Contributors: KG: research project: conception, organisation; statistical analysis: design; manuscript preparation: writing of the first draft; manuscript revision. PB: statistical analysis: review and critique; manuscript preparation: writing of the first draft, review and critique; manuscript revision. KF: statistical analysis: review and critique; manuscript preparation: writing of the first draft, review and critique. SS: statistical analysis: design, execution. ALR-R: statistical analysis: execution; manuscript preparation: review and critique; manuscript revision. FW: research project: conception, organisation. CK: statistical analysis: review and critique; manuscript preparation: review and critique. SB (guarantor): research project: conception, organisation; statistical analysis: design, execution; manuscript preparation: writing of the first draft; manuscript revision.

  • Funding: This work was supported by funding from the Foundation “Else Kröner-Fresenius-Stiftung” within the Else Kröner Graduate School for Medical Students “Jena School for Ageing Medicine (JSAM)”. The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest relevant to this work.We acknowledge support by the German Research Foundation Projekt-Nr. 512648189 and the Open Access Publication Fund of the Thueringer Universitaets- und Landesbibliothek Jena.

  • Competing interests: No, there are no competing interests.

  • Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer-reviewed.

  • Supplemental material: This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.

Data availability statement

Data are available upon reasonable request.

Ethics statements

Patient consent for publication:
Ethics approval:

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Jena University Hospital (code 4572-10/15). Participants gave informed consent to participate in the study before taking part.

  1. close Lewis SJG, Dove A, Robbins TW, et al. Cognitive Impairments in Early Parkinson’s Disease Are Accompanied by Reductions in Activity in Frontostriatal Neural Circuitry. J Neurosci 2003; 23:6351–6.
  2. close Aarsland D, Batzu L, Halliday GM, et al. Parkinson disease-associated cognitive impairment. Nat Rev Dis Primers 2021; 7:47.
  3. close Kehagia AA, Barker RA, Robbins TW, et al. Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: the dual syndrome hypothesis. Neurodegener Dis 2013; 11:79–92.
  4. close Summers D, Spencer K, Okasaki C, et al. An Examination of Cognitive Heterogeneity in Parkinson Disease: The Dual-Syndrome Hypothesis. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2024; 67:1127–35.
  5. close Pourzinal D, Yang J, Sivakumaran K, et al. Longitudinal follow up of data-driven cognitive subtypes in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Behav 2023; 13.
  6. close Del Pino R, Acera M, Murueta-Goyena A, et al. Visual dysfunction is associated with cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2021; 92:22–5.
  7. close Weil RS, Schwarzkopf DS, Bahrami B, et al. Assessing cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: An online tool to detect visuo‐perceptual deficits. Mov Disord 2018; 33:544–53.
  8. close Weil RS, Winston JS, Leyland L-A, et al. Neural correlates of early cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2019; 6:902–12.
  9. close Zarkali A, McColgan P, Leyland LA, et al. Visual Dysfunction Predicts Cognitive Impairment and White Matter Degeneration in Parkinson’s Disease. Mov Disord 2021; 36:1191–202.
  10. close Bundesen C. A theory of visual attention. Psychol Rev 1990; 97:523–47.
  11. close Bublak P, Redel P, Sorg C, et al. Staged decline of visual processing capacity in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2011; 32:1219–30.
  12. close Ruiz-Rizzo AL, Bublak P, Redel P, et al. Simultaneous object perception deficits are related to reduced visual processing speed in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 2017; 55:132–42.
  13. close Finke K, Bublak P, Dose M, et al. Parameter-based assessment of spatial and non-spatial attentional deficits in Huntington’s disease. Brain (Bacau) 2006; 129:1137–51.
  14. close Finke K, Schneider WX, Redel P, et al. The capacity of attention and simultaneous perception of objects: a group study of Huntington’s disease patients. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:3272–84.
  15. close Marié RM, Defer GL. Working memory and dopamine: clinical and experimental clues. Curr Opin Neurol 2003; 16 Suppl 2:S29–35.
  16. close Hutchison CW, Nathan PJ, Mrazek L, et al. Cholinergic modulation of speed of early information processing: the effect of donepezil on inspection time. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 155:440–2.
  17. close Postuma RB, Berg D, Stern M, et al. MDS clinical diagnostic criteria for Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2015; 30:1591–601.
  18. close Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67:361–70.
  19. close Movement Disorder Society Task Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson’s Disease. The Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS): Status and recommendations. Mov Disord 2003; 18:738–50.
  20. close Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology (ECronicon) 1967; 17:427–42.
  21. close Chaudhuri KR, Martinez-Martin P, Schapira AHV, et al. International multicenter pilot study of the first comprehensive self-completed nonmotor symptoms questionnaire for Parkinson’s disease: the NMSQuest study. Mov Disord 2006; 21:916–23.
  22. close Weintraub D, Mamikonyan E, Papay K, et al. Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in Parkinson’s Disease-Rating Scale. Mov Disord 2012; 27:242–7.
  23. close Jenkinson C, Fitzpatrick R, Peto V, et al. The PDQ-8: Development and validation of a short-form parkinson’s disease questionnaire. Psychology & Health 1997; 12:805–14.
  24. close von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, et al. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. J Clin Epidemiol 2008; 61:344–9.
  25. close Martin EM, Rupprecht S, Schrenk S, et al. A hypoarousal model of neurological post-COVID syndrome: the relation between mental fatigue, the level of central nervous activation and cognitive processing speed. J Neurol 2023; 270:4647–60.
  26. close Sperling G. The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 1960; 74:1–29.
  27. close Dyrholm M, Kyllingsbæk S, Espeseth T, et al. Generalizing parametric models by introducing trial-by-trial parameter variability: The case of TVA. J Math Psychol 2011; 55:416–29.
  28. close Bublak P, Redel P, Finke K, et al. Spatial and non-spatial attention deficits in neurodegenerative diseases: assessment based on Bundesen’s theory of visual attention (TVA). Restor Neurol Neurosci 2006; 24:287–301.
  29. close Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53:695–9.
  30. close Litvan I, Goldman JG, Tröster AI, et al. Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Mov Disord 2012; 27:349–56.
  31. close Berthold MR, Cebron N, Dill F, et al. KNIME - the Konstanz information miner: version 2.0 and beyond. ACM SIGKDD Explor Newsl 2009; 11:26–31.
  32. close Ruiz-Rizzo AL, Sorg C, Napiórkowski N, et al. Decreased cingulo-opercular network functional connectivity mediates the impact of aging on visual processing speed. Neurobiol Aging 2019; 73:50–60.
  33. close Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3:201–15.
  34. close Chechlacz M, Gillebert CR, Vangkilde SA, et al. Structural Variability within Frontoparietal Networks and Individual Differences in Attentional Functions: An Approach Using the Theory of Visual Attention. J Neurosci 2015; 35:10647–58.
  35. close Chen B, Fan GG, Liu H, et al. Changes in anatomical and functional connectivity of Parkinson’s disease patients according to cognitive status. Eur J Radiol 2015; 84:1318–24.
  36. close McAvinue LP, Habekost T, Johnson KA, et al. Sustained attention, attentional selectivity, and attentional capacity across the lifespan. Atten Percept Psychophys 2012; 74:1570–82.
  37. close Nielsen S, Wilms LI. Cognitive aging on latent constructs for visual processing capacity: a novel structural equation modeling framework with causal assumptions based on a theory of visual attention. Front Psychol 2014; 5.
  38. close Ahmadi-Abhari S, Guzman-Castillo M, Bandosz P, et al. Temporal trend in dementia incidence since 2002 and projections for prevalence in England and Wales to 2040: modelling study. BMJ 2017; 358.
  39. close Pourzinal D, Yang JHJ, Byrne GJ, et al. Identifying subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease using cluster analysis. J Neurol 2020; 267:3213–22.
  40. close Hu MTM, Szewczyk-Królikowski K, Tomlinson P, et al. Predictors of cognitive impairment in an early stage Parkinson’s disease cohort. Mov Disord 2014; 29:351–9.

  • Received: 28 October 2024
  • Accepted: 21 February 2025
  • First Published: 21 March 2025