Volume 26, Issue 5 e2019
BRIEF REPORT
Full Access

Age-related differences in the relation between the home numeracy environment and numeracy skills

Rebecca J. Thompson

Rebecca J. Thompson

Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, USA

Search for more papers by this author
Amy R. Napoli

Amy R. Napoli

Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, USA

Search for more papers by this author
David J. Purpura

Corresponding Author

David J. Purpura

Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana, USA

Correspondence

David J. Purpura, Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Purdue University, 1202 W. State Street, Rm 231, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907-2055, USA.

Email: purpura@purdue.edu

Search for more papers by this author
First published: 25 January 2017
Citations: 73

Abstract

The home numeracy environment (HNE) is often predictive of children's early mathematics skills, though the findings are mixed. Overall, research on kindergarten-aged children demonstrates a relation between the HNE and early numeracy skills, whereas findings for preschool-aged children are more equivocal. One potential reason for equivocality of these findings is that previous studies have not accounted for the way different practices may relate to children's mathematics skills at different ages. The purpose of the present study was to explore a potential reason for discrepancies in findings of the relation between the HNE and mathematics skills in preschool. Reports of HNE practices were collected from parents of 184 preschool children (71 three year olds and 113 four year olds) and children were assessed on their numeracy skills. Parents of 4-year-olds engaged in HNE activities more frequently than parents of 3-year-olds. Furthermore, more advanced HNE activities were correlated with numeracy performance of older children, but more basic HNE activities were not correlated with numeracy performance of either age group after accounting for parental education. These findings suggest that nuanced approaches in the way the HNE is measured at different ages may be needed in order to accurately assess relations between developmentally appropriate HNE activities and children's outcomes.

Highlights

  • The relation between specific home numeracy environment practices and children's numeracy skills were compared across preschool aged children (3 and 4 years old). Complex home numeracy environment practices were related to numeracy skills of older children, but basic home numeracy environment practices were only related to numeracy skills with younger children until controlling for parental education. More targeted measurement of the home numeracy environment may be needed in order to fully assess its impact on the development of mathematics cognition.

1 INTRODUCTION

Children's early mathematics skills are a strong predictor of later mathematical achievement (Duncan et al., 2007; Jordan, Glutting, & Ramineni, 2010; Pagani, Fitzpatrick, Archambault, & Janosz, 2010). The preschool years, in particular, are a formative period for mathematics development (Arnold, Fisher, Doctoroff, & Dobbs, 2002; Clements & Sarama, 2007; National Mathematics Advisory Panel [NMAP], 2008; Siegler & Ramani, 2008). Children acquire early mathematics skills in multiple settings such as the home or early childhood care centers (NMAP, 2008). In particular, the home numeracy environment (HNE) has often been found to be a significant predictor of children's early numeracy skills (Niklas & Schneider, 2013). However, findings of the relation between the HNE and children's numeracy skills have been mixed, especially at the preschool level. The variation in previous studies may be explained by the combination of two factors: children's rapid development of mathematical skills in the preschool years and parents' reports on their engagement with their child on a number of numeracy-related activities that may be appropriate for some children but not all. Home numeracy practices that are appropriately challenging for a 3-year-old may be too simple for a 4 or 5 year old, whereas age-appropriate practices for a 4-year-old may be too challenging for 3-year-olds. Thus, the purpose of this study was to investigate differential relations between the HNE and numeracy skills for children of different ages during the preschool years.

1.1 The HNE

The HNE is increasingly recognized as an important predictor of children's early numeracy skills (Niklas & Schneider, 2013). In fact, evidence suggests that early home experiences may have a greater influence on educational attainment than the influence of socioeconomic status (Anders et al., 2012; Melhuish et al., 2008). HNE activities can be grouped into two categories: direct and indirect (LeFevre et al., 2009). Direct activities are explicitly mathematics-centered and are used by parents to help their children develop mathematics skills. For example, parent–child engagement in counting objects, practicing number names, and printing numbers are direct HNE practices. Indirect, or informal, HNE activities are tasks or play activities in which the aim is not explicitly to provide mathematics instruction, yet may incidentally engage children's mathematics skills (Missall, Hojnoski, Caskie, & Repasky, 2015). For example, cooking with a child is a task that may incidentally teach mathematical language (add more flour, crack two eggs; Sonnenschein et al., 2012).

1.2 Relations between the HNE and child performance

Despite evidence suggesting that the HNE is related to mathematics skills, findings overall have been mixed across different ages (LeFevre et al., 2009; Manolitsis, Georgiou, & Tziraki, 2013; Missall et al., 2015; Zippert & Ramani, 2016). Specifically, research on kindergarten-aged children often demonstrates a positive relation between the HNE and early mathematics skills (Blevins-Knabe & Musun-Miller, 1996; LeFevre et al., 2009; LeFevre, Polyzoi, Skwarchuk, Fast, & Sowinski, 2010; Niklas & Schneider, 2013), whereas findings for preschool-aged children have been more equivocal (Anders et al., 2012; Blevins-Knabe, Austin, Musun, Eddy, & Jones, 2000; Missall et al., 2015; Ramani, Rowe, Eason, & Leech, 2015; Skwarchuk, 2009). Though findings have been mixed across all age groups, they tend to be most inconsistent for younger preschool-aged children (i.e., 3 and 4-year-olds). The results of several studies of the HNE demonstrating this pattern are summarized in Table 1. Given that differences in children's numeracy skills are present as early as preschool entry (Starkey, Klein, & Wakeley, 2004), it is important to investigate inconsistencies in relations between the HNE and children's outcomes from an early age. Further, positive relations tend to be found primarily for direct numeracy activities but not indirect activities (LeFevre et al., 2010; Manolitsis et al., 2013; cf., LeFevre et al., 2009).

Table 1. Findings of studies relating HNE practices to math performance
Age group Author, year Participant ages Direction of findings Findings
Preschool Anders et al., 2012 Range: 3–6 years

M: 45 months at first assessment

+ Quality of the home numeracy environment was associated with numeracy skills in the first year of preschool.
Blevins-Knabe et al., 2000 Range: 4–6 years n.s. Math activities were not significantly correlated with children's math achievement scores.
DeFlorio & Beliakoff, 2015 Range: 3–4 years +/n.s. Some specific child activities were related to the math composite score, but others were not related.
Missall et al., 2015 Range: 37–69 months

M: 53.6 months

M: 4 years, 5 months

n.s. HNE was not related to early mathematics performance.
Niklas, Cohrssen, & Tayler, 2015 + Children of parents who participated in a quasi-experimental HNE intervention showed greater increases in numeracy outcomes than did children whose parents did not participate in the HNE intervention.
Ramani et al., 2015 Range: 3–5 years

M: 52 months

+/n.s. Controlling for child age, parents' direct teaching of math predicted children's foundational numeracy knowledge but not their advanced numeracy knowledge. Parents' advanced math talk predicted children's advanced numeracy knowledge, but foundational math talk did not predict foundational numeracy knowledge.
Skwarchuk, 2009 Range: 47–64 months

M: 57.76 months

+/− Advanced parent–child numeracy practices positively predicted children's numeracy skills. Basic parent–child numeracy practices were negatively associated with children's numeracy skills.
Zippert & Ramani, 2016 Range: 37–69 months

M: 53 months

+/n.s. Frequency of advanced mathematics was positively correlated with advanced number skills, but basic HNE practiceswere not significantly related to number skills.
Kindergarten and older Blevins-Knabe & Musun-Miller, 1996 Range: 57–77 months

M: 67 months

+/− Four HNE practices were positively related to children's numeracy skills; four HNE practices were negatively related to child's numeracy skills.
LeFevre et al., 2009 Range: 5–7 years

M: K (71 months), grades 1 (81 months) & 2 (93 months)

+/n.s. Playing number games was positively predictive of math knowledge, but practicing number skills, engagement with number books, and indirect math activities were not. Practicing number skills, playing number games, and indirect math activities were positively predictive of math fluency; engagement with number books was not.
LeFevre et al., 2010 M: 70 months + HNE practices were positivelypredictive of numeracy outcomes.
Manolitsis et al., 2013 Range: 5–6 years

M: 64.32 months

+ Parent–child HNE practiceswere positively predictiveof children's math fluency.
Niklas & Schneider, 2013 Range: 63–96 months

M: 77 months

+ The HNE in kindergarten significantly and positively predicted math abilities in first grade (this relation was mediated by children's verbal counting).
  • Note. + indicates positive relations found in the study, − indicates negative relations found in the study, n.s. indicates no significant relations
  • Note. HNE = home numeracy environment.

Findings of the relation between the HNE and mathematics skills in preschool have been more mixed than in kindergarten. Some studies on preschool-aged children have found a relation between the HNE and children's mathematics skills (e.g., Anders et al., 2012; Skwarchuk, 2009). For example, Anders et al. (2012) found that the quality of the home learning environment was strongly associated with numeracy skills in the first year of preschool. In contrast, some studies involving preschool-aged children have found mixed or nonsignificant relations between the HNE and mathematics skills. Ramani et al. (2015), for example, found that parent-reported teaching activities contributed to the prediction of children's foundational mathematics knowledge but not advanced numerical knowledge. Similarly, DeFlorio and Beliakoff (2015) found that some HNE activities were related to children's mathematics performance, but others were not related. Finally, other studies have found that the HNE is not related to mathematics skills (Missall et al., 2015). A potential explanation for these differences across age groups may be that researchers often utilize the same HNE questions for parents of children of various ages. Though the questions are appropriate in general, certain questions may be more appropriate or applicable to specific age groups. As such, it is important to understand how specific HNE questions differentially relate to children of different ages.

1.3 The current study

The mixed findings of the relation between the HNE and children's numeracy performance for preschool-aged children indicate a need to examine the HNE in relation to children's age. In this study, we seek to address the discrepancies in these findings by investigating the relation between the HNE and children's numeracy skills at the item level (i.e., specific parent–child practices rather than the HNE as a whole) in the context of developmental progression of numeracy skills. Specifically, we hypothesized that parents engage in more complex numeracy activities with older preschool children compared to younger children and that HNE activities would be differentially related to numeracy performance at different ages such that more basic HNE practices (e.g., counting and quantity comparison) would be related to younger preschool children's numeracy performance and more advanced practices (e.g., simple sums and numeral naming) would be related to older preschool children's numeracy performance. We examined these hypotheses by (a) comparing the frequency of HNE activities across age groups and (b) identifying the HNE activities that are correlated with numeracy performance for the different age groups (i.e., 3 and 4-year-olds).

2 METHOD

2.1 Participants

One hundred and eighty-four preschool children from 16 preschool centers participated in this study. The study includes 71 three year olds (M age = 3.54, SD = 0.30) and 113 four year olds (M age = 4.43 and SD = 0.28). Participants were approximately representative of the local demographics (56.7% Caucasian, 6.7% African American, 12.2% Hispanic, 7.2% Asian, and 17.2% multiracial). Approximately half (52.7%) of the sample was female. Participants came from a range of socioeconomic status backgrounds as 21.2% of participating parents had a high school degree or less, 32.1% attended some college, and 46.7% had a college degree or higher.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Home numeracy environment

Parents were asked to complete a researcher-created background information questionnaire on characteristics of the family and home environment. Parents also reported the frequency of practicing specific activities in the home with their children, with six options ranging from “never” (0) to “multiple times a day” (5). Items from the questionnaire pertaining to the HNE can be found in the appendix. Questions on the HNE were modified from previous research conducted by LeFevre et al. (2009); differences between the questionnaires are noted in the appendix. Eight questions regarding the frequency of parents' direct numeracy practices were used: counting objects, printing numbers, reading number storybooks, using number activity books, using the terms more and less, counting down, learning simple sums, and identifying written numbers. Parents also reported the frequency of 10 indirect numeracy practices including: using calendars and dates, measuring ingredients while cooking, playing board games with a die or spinner, connect-the-dot activities, counting out money, making and sorting collections, playing store; playing card games, sorting things by size, color, or shape, and talking about money while shopping. A direct activities composite variable (α = .80) was created by calculating an average of the direct activities; and an indirect activities composite variable (α = .83) was created by calculating an average of the indirect activities.

2.2.2 Early numeracy

The Preschool Early Numeracy Skills Test–Brief Version (PENS-B; Purpura, Reid, Eiland, & Baroody, 2015) is a 24-item numeracy task (α = .93). The 24 items are representative of the broad numeracy skills children are expected to acquire in preschool and kindergarten and take approximately 5 min to administer. One point is given for each correct answer and a 3-in-a-row ceiling rule is applied. Furthermore, the measure has evidence of convergent and discriminant validity (Purpura et al., 2015).

2.3 Procedure

2.3.1 Assessment procedure

The HNE was assessed through a survey that was sent home to parents along with the consent form. Children were assessed on the numeracy task as part of a larger assessment battery in the fall of the academic year. Assessments took place in the local preschools at times identified by the schools in a room or an area designated by the school directors or teachers. Individuals who had either completed or were working toward completion of a Bachelor's degree in social sciences fields conducted the assessments. All testers completed two 2–3 hour training sessions, followed by individual practice, and a “testing-out” session to ensure they were fluent with the assessment measures and general principles of working with children.

2.3.2 Analytic procedure

To determine whether the frequency of parent–child practices was different for parents of 3-year-olds and parents of 4-year-olds, we conducted t tests to compare differences by age group on the frequency of engaging in 18 individual HNE activities, as well as the two composites (direct HNE practices and indirect HNE practices). To examine if different HNE practices were related to numeracy performance at different ages, we calculated bivariate correlations between the individual HNE variables and PENS-B scores separately for 3-year-olds and 4-year-olds. Partial correlations (accounting for parental education) were also calculated in this analysis. Of the 184 participants, 156 had valid PENS-B scores and could be analyzed in the correlational analyses.

3 RESULTS

3.1 Differences in home numeracy activities at 3 and 4 years of age

3.1.1 Overall frequencies of practices

Of all 18 activities, parents engaged their children most frequently in counting objects, reporting an average of 2 to 5 times per week for both age groups. All other HNE practices were practiced less than 2 to 5 times per week. Overall, direct activities were reported more frequently than indirect activities with direct activities on average occurring at least weekly and indirect activities occurring only a couple of times a month on average. Among direct activities, the least frequently reported activity was learning simple sums, which occurred an average of 1 to 3 times per month. All indirect activities were reported to occur between 1 to 3 times a month or about once a week, with the exception of sorting things by size, color, or shape, which was reported to occur slightly more than about once a week or more.

3.1.2 Age-related differences in practices

As shown in Figure 1 and Table 2, parents of 4-year-olds engaged in HNE activities with their child more frequently than did parents of 3-year-olds for broad measures of both direct (p = .042, d = 0.31) and indirect activities (p = .011, d = 0.39). Among direct items, parents of 4-year-olds did the following with their child more frequently than the parents of 3-year-olds: printing numbers (p = .004, d = 0.61), using number activity books (p = .028, d = 0.33), and learning simple sums (p = .006, d = 0.42). Among the indirect items, parents of 4-year-olds did the following activities with their child more frequently than the parents of 3-year-olds: counting out money (p = .026, d = 0.34); sorting things by size, color, or shape (p = .037, d = 0.16); and talking about money while shopping (p = .008, d = 0.41).

Details are in the caption following the image
Means of specific home numeracy environment (HNE) activities across ages. HNE scores are based on the following scale: 0 = never, 1 = one to three times per month, 2 = about once a week, 3 = two to five times per week, 4 = daily, 5 = multiple times a day. Note. Numbers included in this graph are not equal intervals (e.g., 4 does not indicate twice the activity as 2). *p < .05
Table 2. Frequency of HNE activities at different ages
3-year-olds (n = 71) 4-year-olds (n = 113)
Task M SD M SD p
Direct activities
Direct activities composite 2.02 0.81 2.29 0.92 .042
Counting objects 3.57 0.88 3.46 0.96 .440
Printing numbers 1.50 1.32 2.10 0.44 .004
Reading number storybooks 2.66 1.09 2.39 1.39 .162
Using number activity books 1.61 1.21 2.02 1.30 .028
Using the terms “more or less” 2.18 1.70 2.63 1.55 .061
Counting down 1.57 1.57 2.02 1.45 .052
Learning simple sums 1.04 1.24 1.58 1.33 .006
Identifying the names of written numbers 2.01 1.57 2.10 1.51 .710
Indirect activities
Indirect activities composite 1.36 0.76 1.67 0.84 .011
Using calendars and dates 1.49 1.41 1.85 1.53 .107
Measuring ingredients while cooking 1.32 1.31 1.59 1.35 .184
Playing board games with die or spinner 1.04 1.10 1.30 1.24 .140
Connect-the-dot activities 1.17 1.18 1.37 1.18 .272
Counting out money 1.04 1.20 1.47 1.33 .026
Making and sorting collections 1.84 1.39 2.01 1.45 .421
Playing store 1.47 1.40 1.86 1.40 .069
Playing card games 1.20 1.02 1.38 1.18 .289
Sorting things by size, color, shape 2.16 2.23 2.55 2.54 .037
Talking about money while shopping 1.01 1.18 1.53 1.36 .008
  • N = 184. Home numeracy environment (HNE) scores are based on the following scale: 0 = never, 1 = one to three times per month, 2 = about once a month, 3 = two to five times per week, 4 = daily, and 5 = multiple times a day.

3.2 Relations between specific HNE activities and mathematics performance

As shown in Table 3, neither the broad measure of direct activities nor the broad measure of indirect activities were related to 3-year-olds' PENS-B scores. However, the broad measure of direct activities was related to 4-year-olds' PENS-B scores. For 3-year-olds, the direct activities of counting objects (r = .30, p = .025), using the terms “more or less” (r = .30, p = .024), and counting out money (r = −.37, p = .007) were correlated with PENS-B scores. However, when partial correlation analyses were conducted accounting for parental education (scored as a dichotomous variable of “less than a 2-year college degree” or “2-year college degree or higher”), the relation between these variables was no longer significant. For 4-year-olds, counting down (r = .20, p = .047), learning simple sums (r = .27, p = .006), and identifying names of written numbers (r = .26, p = .009) were positively correlated with PENS-B scores. The same items were significant when partial correlations accounting for parental education were calculated. Importantly, there were no HNE activities that were significantly correlated with PENS-B scores for both 3 and 4-year-olds.

Table 3. Correlation between HNE activities and PENS-B by age
Task 3-year-olds (n = 57) 4-year-olds (n = 99)
r Partial r r Partial r
Direct activities
Direct activities composite .17 .10 .23* .24*
 Counting objects .30* .20 .15 .13
 Printing numbers −.01 −.09 .10 .08
 Reading number storybooks .13 .15 .09 .08
 Using number activity books −.16 −.09 −.03 .03
 Using the terms “more or less” .30* .19 .17 .18
 Counting down .00 −.02 .20** .22*
 Learning simple sums .21 .18 .27** .33**
 Identifying the names of written numbers .07 .00 .26** .22*
Indirect activities
 Indirect activities composite −.05 .00 −.10 .00
 Using calendars and dates −.10 −.06 .16 .16
 Measuring ingredients while cooking .15 .23 −.07 −.04
 Playing board games with die or spinner .01 −.13 .02 .09
 Connect-the-dot activities .06 . 05 −.06 −.02
 Counting out money −.37** −.25 −.15 −.13
 Making and sorting collections .08 .07 .10 .14
 Playing store −.02 .05 −.19 −.06
 Playing card games −.01 −.04 −.09 −.04
 Sorting things by size, color, shape .02 .09 −.02 .10
 Talking about money while shopping −.23 −.17 −.12 .06
  • N = 156. Note. Partial correlations account for parental education.
  • * p < .05
  • ** p < .01

4 DISCUSSION

The findings of this study indicate that parents of 4-year-olds engage their children in more numeracy activities overall, as well as more complex activities, than do parents of 3-year-olds. Furthermore, specific aspects of the HNE are differentially related to mathematics performance at specific ages. These relations appear to align with a general learning trajectories framework wherein children's early mathematics skills are cumulative and build on each other, becoming more complex over time (Sarama & Clements, 2009). These findings provide a plausible explanation for why previous findings on the relation between the HNE and mathematics performance are mixed in the preschool years. Namely, some prior work may have used items that were not aligned with age-based practices. Age appropriate measures of the HNE may be needed to best capture the variance in mathematics activities that parents do with their children.

4.1 Frequency of engagement in HNE activities

Overall, parent–child HNE engagement was quite low for both 3 and 4-year-olds. Ten of the 18 activities only occurred about once per month. Though parents of 4-year-olds engaged their children overall in more HNE activities than parents of 3-year-olds, these differences were not found for all HNE activities. Rather, these differences were found for specific activities that may be categorized as more complex, such as learning simple sums. This finding suggests that the greater amount of engagement in HNE activities with older children does not reflect an overall increase in HNE activities, but rather an adding on of more complex activities. In considering how the HNE influences children's numeracy development, researchers should consider the mathematical learning trajectory and where specific HNE activities fall on this trajectory.

4.2 Relations between HNE activities and mathematics performance

The composite measure of the direct HNE practices was related to PENS-B scores of 4-year-olds but not of 3-year-olds. This relation may be due to the greater frequency of engagement in complex direct activities for 4-year-olds compared with 3-year-olds. Indirect activities, as a whole, were not related to PENS-B scores of either age group, a finding that is consistent with previous research (LeFevre et al., 2010; Manolitsis et al., 2013). It is possible that indirect activities were not related to PENS-B scores (at either the composite or individual level) because they occurred so infrequently, rarely more than about once per week. Further, the lack of relation may be because parents did not take advantage of the opportunity that indirect activities present for mathematically-focused dialogue but focused instead on different aspects of the activities (e.g., cultural relevance; Vandermaas-Peeler, Nelson, & Bumpass, 2007). Further research is needed to examine differences in parent–child practices related to quantity and quality of practices and how that distinction relates to improving children's mathematical performance.

As expected, different types of individual HNE activities were associated with PENS-B scores for the different age groups. Specifically, more basic activities were correlated with PENS-B scores of 3-year-olds (though this relation was attenuated after controlling for parental education) and more complex activities were correlated with PENS-B scores of 4-year-olds. These findings indicate that parent–child numeracy activities are not consistently associated with numeracy performance across the preschool years. It may be that correlates “advance” along the learning trajectory of preschoolers as children advance in their ability levels. Importantly, none of the same HNE practices were correlated with numeracy outcomes of both 3 and 4-year-olds. This differential relation suggests that there is significant development in mathematical competencies within a year, an idea that is consistent with results of previous studies (Arnold et al., 2002; Clements & Sarama, 2007; NMAP, 2008; Siegler & Ramani, 2008). However, it must be noted that when parental education was accounted for in the relation between HNE items and numeracy skills, no items were significantly related to numeracy performance for 3-year-olds, but the same items were still related for 4-year-olds. This finding suggests that the relation may be driven by parental education, where children from families with higher parental education may engage in more mathematical activities than children from families with lower parental education, particularly at younger ages.

Given the prior mixed findings on the relation between the HNE and mathematics performance in preschool, it may be that the items did not align specifically with the age or developmental level of children in some of the samples. In contrast, more commonly practiced activities may be those that align with the age or ability level of older preschool and kindergarten age children. This alignment may explain why there is more consistency in relations with older children. Note that three of the most commonly practiced items for 4-year-olds were the ones that were significantly related to mathematics performance. Ultimately, it may be necessary to design HNE measures that vary by age so as to evaluate the most relevant practices for a child's age and expected ability level.

4.3 Limitations

A few limitations of this study should be noted. First, the study is correlational in nature and the two age groups were assessed concurrently, rather than assessing the same participants at 3 and 4 years of age. As such, causal claims cannot be made about the effect of HNE practices on mathematics performance. Second, this study used caregiver report to assess the HNE. Although caregiver report is often used to measure the home environment, research that uses diary methods or recordings of parent–child interactions may help to provide a better assessment of the frequency, and particularly the quality, with which parents engage in different activities in the home. In addition, caregiver report may be subject to reporter bias. Third, the list of HNE practices was limited. Additional HNE practices, such as identifying which of two numbers is larger, and a more detailed breakdown of included practices should also be examined in future research. As the items used in this study were broadly defined, definite conclusions cannot be drawn regarding whether an item should be regarded as basic or complex and whether or not these more narrowly specified items would be related to performance at different ages. For example, the current item “counting objects” could be modified to be “counting small quantities of objects (five objects or fewer)” and “counting larger quantities of objects (more than five objects).” Finally, the PENS-B assesses children's general numeracy abilities. Though understanding the relations between specific aspects of the HNE and children's broad numeracy skills is useful, future research should include assessments of children's specific numeracy outcomes. For example, it is important to understand the relation between the frequency of parents working with children to identify written numerals and children's numeral identification skills.

4.4 Implications and future directions

The findings indicate that further research on the HNE may benefit from utilizing different HNE activities to assess children of different ages. Though questionnaires that are widely used to assess the HNE (e.g., the measure utilized in the LeFevre et al. 2010 study) are appropriate for certain age groups, they may not be appropriate to understand the HNE of all children. When HNE activities are being used as predictors of numeracy performance, they should account for the changes in children's mathematical abilities across the given time. Further research is needed to determine which specific HNE practices are not only the strongest correlates, but also the strongest predictors of numeracy performance at each age. Selecting age-appropriate HNE practices may enable research in this area to more accurately reflect the relations between parent–child mathematics engagement and children's numeracy performance across the developmental spectrum.

5 CONCLUSION

The results of this study indicate that parents of 4-year-olds practice numeracy activities with their children more frequently than do parents of 3-year-olds. Additionally, results demonstrate that the relation between HNE activities and numeracy performance is both age- and activity-specific. This study highlights the need to focus on different HNE activities at different ages and to utilize developmentally appropriate items when assessing children's HNE in order to more accurately understand and assess the relations between the HNE and young children's numeracy skills.

APPENDIX A:

Home numeracy environment questionnaire

Parents were asked, “In the past month, how often did you and your child engage in the following activities?” and instructed to mark the appropriate box (Table A1). Although activities are separated here by direct and indirect practices, they were not labeled or separated on the questionnaire that parents received. The home numeracy environment questionnaire was modified from previous research (LeFevre et al., 2009). The following items were included on the LeFevre et al. (2010) questionnaire but were not included in this study: using number or arithmetic flashcards, being timed, having your child wear a watch, playing with calculators, and playing with number fridge magnets. Additionally, the question regarding collections (Item 14) was modified from “making collections.” Finally, Items 5 and 13 were not included in the LeFevre et al. (2010) study but were added to the current study to expand coverage of age-appropriate skills.

Table A1. Home numeracy environment questionnaire
Never 1 to 3 times a month About once a week 2–5 times per week Daily Multiple times per day
Direct practices
1. Counting objects
2. Printing numbers
3. Reading number storybooks
4. Using number activity books
5. Using the terms more/less
6. Counted down (10, 9, 8, 7. . )
7. Learning simple sums (i.e., 2 + 2 = ? )
8. Identify names of written numbers
Indirect practices
9. Using calendars and dates
10. Measuring ingredients when cooking
11. Playing board games with die or spinner
12. “connect-the-dot” activities
13. Counting out money
14. Making/sorting collections
15. Playing “store”
16. Playing card games
17. Sort things by size, color or shape
18. Talk about money when shopping (“which costs more?”)

    Citation Statements
    • Supporting
      Supporting4
    • Mentioning
      Mentioning84
    • Contrasting
      Contrasting1
    Explore this article's citation statements on scite.ai

    The full text of this article hosted at iucr.org is unavailable due to technical difficulties.